Oni Science
  • Home
  • Environment
  • Humans
  • Nature
  • Physics
  • Space
  • Tech
  • Video
  • Contact Us
    • About us
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms and Conditions
    • Amazon Disclaimer
    • DMCA / Copyrights Disclaimer
Skip to content
Oni Science
Your Daily Science News
  • Environment
  • Humans
  • Nature
  • Physics
  • Space
  • Tech
  • Video
  • Contact Us
    • About us
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms and Conditions
    • Amazon Disclaimer
    • DMCA / Copyrights Disclaimer
Nature

Scientists Have Figured Out How Honeybees Learn Their Waggle Dance Moves

March 11, 2023 by admin 0 Comments

Share on Facebook
Share on Twitter
Share on Pinterest
Share on LinkedIn

The Greek historian Herodotus reported over 2,000 years ago on a misguided forbidden experiment in which two children were prevented from hearing human speech so that a king could discover the true, unlearned language of human beings.

Scientists now know that human language requires social learning and interaction with other people, a property shared with multiple animal languages.

But why should humans and other animals need to learn a language instead of being born with this knowledge, like many other animal species?

This question fascinates me and my colleagues and is the basis for our recent paper published in the journal Science. As a biologist, I have spent decades studying honeybee communication and how it may have evolved.

There are two common answers to why language should be learned or innate.

For one, complex languages can often respond to local conditions as they are learned. A second answer is that complex communication is often difficult to produce even when individuals are born with some knowledge of the correct signals.

Given that the ways honeybees communicate are quite elaborate, we decided to study how they learn these behaviors to answer this language question.

What is a waggle dance?

Astonishingly, honeybees possess one of the most complicated examples of nonhuman communication.

They can tell each other where to find resources such as food, water, or nest sites with a physical “waggle dance.” This dance conveys the direction, distance, and quality of a resource to the bee’s nestmates.

Essentially, the dancer points recruits in the correct direction and tells them how far to go by repeatedly circling around in a figure-eight pattern centered around a waggle run, in which the bee waggles its abdomen as it moves forward.

frameborder=”0″ allow=”accelerometer; autoplay; clipboard-write; encrypted-media; gyroscope; picture-in-picture; web-share” allowfullscreen>

Dancers are pursued by potential recruits, bees that closely follow the dancer, to learn where to go to find the communicated resource.

Longer waggle runs communicate greater distances, and the waggle angle communicates direction. For higher-quality resources such as sweeter nectar, dancers repeat the waggle run more times and race back faster after each waggle run.

Making mistakes

This dance is difficult to produce. The dancer is not only running – covering about one body length per second – while trying to maintain the correct waggle angle and duration.

It is also usually in total darkness, amid a crowd of jostling bees and on an irregular surface.

Bees therefore can make three different types of mistakes: pointing in the wrong direction, signaling the wrong distance, or making more errors in performing the figure eight dance pattern – what researchers call disorder errors.

The first two mistakes make it harder for recruits to find the location being communicated. Disorder errors may make it harder for recruits to follow the dancer.

Scientists knew that all bees of the species Apis mellifera begin to forage and dance only as they get older and that they also follow experienced dancers before they first attempt to dance. Could they be learning from practiced teachers?

A ‘forbidden’ bee experiment

My colleagues and I thus created isolated experimental colonies of bees that could not observe other waggle dances before they themselves danced.

Like the ancient experiment described by Herodotus, these bees could not observe the dance language because they were all the same age and had no older, experienced bees to follow.

In contrast, our control colonies contained bees of all ages, so younger bees could follow the older, experienced dancers.

We recorded the first dances of bees living in colonies with both population age profiles.

The bees that could not follow the dances of experienced bees produced dances with significantly more directional, distance, and disorder errors than the dances of control novice bees.

We then tested the same bees later, when they were experienced foragers. Bees who had lacked teachers now produced significantly fewer directional and disorder errors, possibly because they had more practice or had learned by eventually following other dancers.

The dances of the older control bees from colonies with teachers remained just as good as their first dances.

This finding told us that bees are therefore born with some knowledge of how to dance, but they can learn how to dance even better by following experienced bees.

This is the first known example of such complex social learning of communication in insects and is a form of animal culture.

Dance dialects are about distance

A mystery remained with respect to the bees that had lacked dance teachers early on. They could never correct their distance errors. They continued to overshoot, communicating greater distances than normal.

So, why is this interesting to scientists? The answer may lie in how distance communication could adapt to local conditions.

There can be significant differences in where food is distributed in different environments. As a result, different honeybee species have evolved different “dance dialects,” described as the relationship between the distance to a food source and the corresponding waggle dance duration.

Interestingly, these dialects vary, even within the same honeybee species. Researchers suspect this variation exists because colonies, even of the same species, can live in very different environments.

If learning language is a way to cope with different environments, then perhaps each colony should have a distance dialect tailored to its locale and passed on from experienced bees to novices.

If so, our teacher-deprived individual bees may never have corrected their distance errors because they acquired, on their own, a different distance dialect.

Normally, this dialect would be learned from experienced bees, but could potentially change within a single generation if their environmental conditions changed or if the colony swarmed to a new location.

In addition, each colony has a “dance floor,” or the space where bees dance, with complex terrain that the dancers may learn to better navigate over time or by following in the footsteps of older dancers.

These ideas remain to be tested but provide a foundation for future experiments that will explore cultural transmission between older and younger bees.

We believe that this study and future studies will expand our understanding of collective knowledge and language learning in animal societies.The Conversation

James C. Nieh, Associate Dean and Professor of Biology, University of California, San Diego

This article is republished from The Conversation under a Creative Commons license. Read the original article.

This article was originally published by Sciencealert.com. Read the original article here.

Articles You May Like

Tasmanian Tiger ‘Probably’ Survived to 1980s or Even Later, Study Claims
Baby Planets May Do Something Sneaky With Their Water to Protect It From Unruly Stars
Complete Depiction of The Zodiac Found in Ancient Egyptian Temple
Scientists Discover RNA Component Buried in The Dust of an Asteroid
AI Could Be Our Best Chance of Finding Life on Mars. Here’s Why.

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Recent Articles

  • Newly Discovered Species of Orchid Looks Like Delicate Piece of Glass Art
  • This Incredible Flower Makes Fake Flies, And We Finally Know How
  • Complete Depiction of The Zodiac Found in Ancient Egyptian Temple
  • Radical NASA Propulsion Concept Could Reach Interstellar Space in Under 5 Years
  • Don’t Be Fooled: The Hidden Detail NASA Didn’t Show in New Spacesuits
  • Octopus Farming Is Deeply Disturbing. A Professor Explains Why.
  • Tasmanian Tiger ‘Probably’ Survived to 1980s or Even Later, Study Claims
  • NASA Is Tracking a Huge, Growing Anomaly in Earth’s Magnetic Field
  • ‘Giant Hole’ in The Sun Predicted to Unleash Stunning Light Show Across US
  • Physicists Have Manipulated ‘Quantum Light’ For The First Time, in a Huge Breakthrough

Space

  • Radical NASA Propulsion Concept Could Reach Interstellar Space in Under 5 Years
  • Don’t Be Fooled: The Hidden Detail NASA Didn’t Show in New Spacesuits
  • NASA Is Tracking a Huge, Growing Anomaly in Earth’s Magnetic Field
  • ‘Giant Hole’ in The Sun Predicted to Unleash Stunning Light Show Across US
  • Strange Acceleration of Mysterious Interstellar Visitor Finally Explained

Physics

  • Physicists Have Manipulated ‘Quantum Light’ For The First Time, in a Huge Breakthrough
  • ‘Ghost Particles’: Scientists Finally Detect Neutrinos in Particle Collider
  • We’re Either Suspiciously Lucky, or There Really Are Many Universes Out There
  • Blueprint of a Quantum Wormhole Teleporter Could Point to Deeper Physics
  • ‘Time Reflections’ Finally Observed by Physicists After Decades of Searching

Archives

  • March 2023
  • February 2023
  • January 2023
  • December 2022
  • November 2022
  • October 2022
  • September 2022
  • August 2022
  • July 2022
  • September 2020
  • August 2020
  • July 2020
  • June 2020
  • May 2020
  • April 2020
  • October 2019
  • September 2019
  • August 2019
  • July 2019
  • June 2019
  • May 2019
  • April 2019
  • March 2019
  • February 2019
  • January 2019
  • December 2018
  • November 2018
  • October 2018
  • September 2018
  • August 2018
  • July 2018
  • June 2018
  • May 2018
  • April 2018
  • March 2018
  • February 2018
  • January 2018
  • December 2017
  • November 2017
  • September 2017
  • August 2017
  • March 2017
  • November 2016

Categories

  • Environment
  • Humans
  • Nature
  • Physics
  • Space
  • Tech
  • Video

Useful Links

  • Contact Us
  • About us
  • Privacy Policy
  • Terms and Conditions
  • Amazon Disclaimer
  • DMCA / Copyrights Disclaimer

Recent Posts

  • Newly Discovered Species of Orchid Looks Like Delicate Piece of Glass Art
  • This Incredible Flower Makes Fake Flies, And We Finally Know How
  • Complete Depiction of The Zodiac Found in Ancient Egyptian Temple
  • Radical NASA Propulsion Concept Could Reach Interstellar Space in Under 5 Years
  • Don’t Be Fooled: The Hidden Detail NASA Didn’t Show in New Spacesuits

Copyright © 2023 by Oni Science. All rights reserved. All articles, images, product names, logos, and brands are property of their respective owners. All company, product and service names used in this website are for identification purposes only. Use of these names, logos, and brands does not imply endorsement unless specified. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy.

Powered by WordPress using DisruptPress Theme.